首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
Rapid eye movements and remembering   总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6  
J A Empson  P R Clarke 《Nature》1970,227(5255):287-288
  相似文献   

4.
5.
Alpha rhythm and eye movements in eidetic imagery   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
D A Pollen  M C Trachtenberg 《Nature》1972,237(5350):109-112
  相似文献   

6.
The perception of heading during eye movements.   总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5  
C S Royden  M S Banks  J A Crowell 《Nature》1992,360(6404):583-585
When a person walks through a rigid environment while holding eyes and head fixed, the pattern of retinal motion flows radially away from a point, the focus of expansion (Fig. 1a). Under such conditions of translation, heading corresponds to the focus of expansion and people identify it readily. But when making an eye/head movement to track an object off to the side, retinal motion is no longer radial (Fig. 1b). Heading perception in such situations has been modelled in two ways. Extra-retinal models monitor the velocity of rotational movements through proprioceptive or efference information from the extraocular and neck muscles and use that information to discount rotation effects. Retinal-image models determine (and eliminate) rotational components from the retinal image alone. These models have been tested by measuring heading perception under two conditions. First, observers judged heading while tracking a point on a simulated ground plane. Second, they fixated a stationary point and the flow field simulated the effects of a tracking eye movement. Extra-retinal models predict poorer performance in the simulated condition because the eyes do not move. Retinal-image models predict no difference in performance because the two conditions produce identical patterns of retinal motion. Warren and Hannon observed similar performance and concluded that people do not require extra-retinal information to judge heading with eye/head movements present, but they used extremely slow tracking eye movements of 0.2-1.2 deg s-1; a moving observer frequently tracks objects at much higher rates (L. Stark, personal communication). Here we examine heading judgements at higher, more typical eye movement velocities and find that people require extra-retinal information about eye position to perceive heading accurately under many viewing conditions.  相似文献   

7.
8.
9.
本文针对对空透水成像图像的对比度增强问题,提出一种改进的直方图均衡化快速算法。在较好地突出图像细节信息、消除块状效应的同时,避免了复杂的数学运算,取得较好的效果。  相似文献   

10.
Rapid eye movements and dream imagery: are they related?   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
E Moskowitz  R J Berger 《Nature》1969,224(5219):613-614
  相似文献   

11.
Through the development of a high-acuity fovea, primates with frontal eyes have acquired the ability to use binocular eye movements to track small objects moving in space. The smooth-pursuit system moves both eyes in the same direction to track movement in the frontal plane (frontal pursuit), whereas the vergence system moves left and right eyes in opposite directions to track targets moving towards or away from the observer (vergence tracking). In the cerebral cortex and brainstem, signals related to vergence eye movements--and the retinal disparity and blur signals that elicit them--are coded independently of signals related to frontal pursuit. Here we show that these types of signal are represented in a completely different way in the smooth-pursuit region of the frontal eye fields. Neurons of the frontal eye field modulate strongly during both frontal pursuit and vergence tracking, which results in three-dimensional cartesian representations of eye movements. We propose that the brain creates this distinctly different intermediate representation to allow these neurons to function as part of a system that enables primates to track and manipulate objects moving in three-dimensional space.  相似文献   

12.
13.
C Lee  W H Rohrer  D L Sparks 《Nature》1988,332(6162):357-360
The deeper layers of the superior colliculus are involved in the initiation and execution of saccadic (high velocity) eye movements. A large population of coarsely tuned collicular neurons is active before each saccade. The mechanisms by which the signals that precisely control the direction and amplitude of a saccade are extracted from the activity of the population are unknown. It has been assumed that the exact trajectory of a saccade is determined by the activity of the entire population and that information is not extracted from only the most active cells in the population at a subsequent stage of neural processing. The trajectory of a saccade could be based on vector summation of the movement tendencies provided by each member of the population of active neurons or be determined by a weighted average of the vector contributions of each neuron in the active population. Here we present the results of experiments in which a small subset of the active population was reversibly deactivated with lidocaine. These results are consistent with the predictions of the latter population-averaging hypothesis and support the general idea that the direction, amplitude and velocity of saccadic eye movements are based on the responses of the entire population of cells active before a saccadic eye movement.  相似文献   

14.
D O Bahcall  E Kowler 《Nature》1999,400(6747):864-866
A central problem in human vision is to explain how the visual world remains stable despite the continual displacements of the retinal image produced by rapid saccadic movements of the eyes. Perceived stability has been attributed to 'efferent-copy' signals, representing the saccadic motor commands, that cancel the effects of saccade-related retinal displacements. Here we show, by means of a perceptual illusion, that traditional cancellation theories cannot explain stability. The perceptual illusion was produced by first inducing adaptive changes in saccadic gain (ratio of saccade size to target eccentricity). Following adaptation, subjects experienced an illusory mislocalization in which widely separated targets flashed before and after saccades appeared to be in the same place. The illusion shows that the perceptual system did not take the adaptive changes into account. Perceptual localization is based on signals representing the size of the initially-intended saccade, not the size of the saccade that is ultimately executed. Signals representing intended saccades initiate a visual comparison process used to maintain perceptual stability across saccades and to generate the oculomotor error signals that ensure saccadic accuracy.  相似文献   

15.
16.
The Cr:Fe ratio (chromium-to-iron mass ratio) of chromite affects the production of chrome-based ferroalloys. Although the literature contains numerous reports related to the magnetic separation of different minerals, limited work concerning the application of magnetic separation to fine chromite from the Sukinda region of India to enhance its Cr:Fe ratio has been reported. In the present investigation, magnetic separation and mineralogical characterization studies of chromite fines were conducted to enhance the Cr:Fe ratio. Characterization studies included particle size and chemical analyses, X-ray diffraction analysis, automated mineral analysis, sink-and-float studies, and magnetic susceptibility measurements, whereas magnetic separation was investigated using a rare earth drum magnetic separator, a rare earth roll magnetic separator, an induced roll magnetic separator, and a wet high-intensity magnetic separator. The fine chromite was observed to be upgraded to a Cr:Fe ratio of 2.2 with a yield of 55.7% through the use of an induced roll magnetic separator and a feed material with a Cr:Fe ratio of 1.6.  相似文献   

17.
Tanaka M  Lisberger SG 《Nature》2001,409(6817):191-194
In studies of the neural mechanisms giving rise to behaviour, changes in the neural and behavioural responses produced by a given stimulus have been widely reported. This 'gain control' can boost the responses to sensory inputs that are particularly relevant, select among reflexes for execution by motoneurons or emphasize specific movement targets. Gain control is also an integral part of the smooth-pursuit eye movement system. One signature of gain control is that a brief perturbation of a stationary target during fixation causes tiny eye movements, whereas the same perturbation of a moving target during the active state of accurate pursuit causes large responses. Here we show that electrical stimulation of the smooth-pursuit eye movement region in the arcuate sulcus of the frontal lobe ('the frontal pursuit area', FPA) mimics the active state of pursuit. Such stimulation enhances the response to a brief perturbation of target motion, regardless of the direction of motion. We postulate that the FPA sets the gain of pursuit, thereby participating in target selection for pursuit.  相似文献   

18.
19.
初中不同阅读能力学生的阅读眼动特点比较   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
用眼动记录仪对初中二年级不同阅读能力的学生(高阅读能力和低阅读能力各10名)在阅读说明文时的眼动过程进行了记录.结果发现:高阅读能力学生在阅读时间、注视点的持续时间、回视次数和注视次数上都显著地低于低阅读能力的学生,而注视频率显著地高于低阅读能力的学生;在眼跳角度和眼跳潜伏期2项眼动指标上没有显著差异.  相似文献   

20.
天童常绿阔叶林次生演替过程中细根的生物量动态   总被引:2,自引:1,他引:2  
应用土钻法对天童常绿阔叶林次生演替过程中细根的生物量进行了调查,结果显示:细根生物量随演替进程变化显著(P<0.05),在演替前期的灌丛阶段达最高(3.286 t·hm-2),在演替中期马尾松和马尾松+木荷群落降低(1.066 t·hm-2和1.531 t·hm-2),到演替后期的木荷群落和栲树群落再次升高(2.715 t·hm-2和3.181 t·hm-2).各演替阶段活细根和死细根生物量的季节变化显著(P<0.01).各群落的活细根生物量在7月达最高,而在11月最低;死细根生物量,各群落皆于11月最小,而在演替早期两群落和马尾松+木荷群落4月最大而其它群落7月最大.各演替阶段细根生物量随着土层深度增加而下降明显,细根生物量80%左右分布在20 cm以上土层.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号