首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 46 毫秒
1.
E A DeYoe  D C Van Essen 《Nature》1985,317(6032):58-61
V2 is a visual area of the macaque monkey which is at the second level in a recently proposed hierarchy of cortical visual areas. Histochemical staining for cytochrome oxidase (CO) in V2 reveals a pattern of alternate thick and thin CO-rich stripes separated by CO-sparse interstripes. These subregions receive distinct inputs from neurones in CO-rich and CO-sparse zones arrayed within the superficial layers of V1 (refs 4, 5). Are output projections from V2 to higher visual areas also segregated? Using an anatomical double-label paradigm, we have now demonstrated that V2 cells projecting to two of its major target areas, MT and V4 (refs 6, 7), are arranged in stripe-like clusters which are largely segregated from one another and which are closely related to the pattern of CO stripes. Concomitant electrophysiological recordings from V2 indicate that groups of cells having similar receptive field properties are clustered within the subregions defined by these anatomical techniques.  相似文献   

2.
Chatterjee S  Callaway EM 《Nature》2003,426(6967):668-671
The trichromatic primate retina parses the colour content of a visual scene into 'red/green' and 'blue/yellow' representations. Cortical circuits must combine the information encoded in these colour-opponent signals to reconstruct the full range of perceived colours. Red/green and blue/yellow inputs are relayed by the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) of thalamus to primary visual cortex (V1), so understanding how cortical circuits transform these signals requires understanding how LGN inputs to V1 are organized. Here we report direct recordings from LGN afferent axons in muscimol-inactivated V1. We found that blue/yellow afferents terminated exclusively in superficial cortical layers 3B and 4A, whereas red/green afferents were encountered only in deeper cortex, in lower layer 4C. We also describe a distinct cortical target for 'blue-OFF' cells, whose afferents terminated in layer 4A and seemed patchy in organization. The more common 'blue-ON' afferents were found in 4A as well as lower layer 2/3. Chromatic information is thus conveyed to V1 by parallel, anatomically segregated colour-opponent systems, to be combined at a later stage of the colour circuit.  相似文献   

3.
Long-range colour-generating interactions across the retina   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
E P?ppel 《Nature》1986,320(6062):523-525
The existence of colour-generating interactions across the corpus callosum has recently been suggested from observations with a 'split-brain' patient, thus indicating long-range colour computations at the cortical level. Observations on induced colours described here suggest long-range colour computations at the retinal level. If a white surface surrounded by a particular colour is fixated for some time, the resulting after-image has two colours: the surround appears in complementary colour, whereas the white centre takes on the colour of the surround. The question of whether such colour induction is located in the retina or more centrally was tested in a brain-injured patient with hemianopia. It could be demonstrated that areas of the visual field that are no longer represented in the geniculo-striatal pathway still contribute to colour induction, suggesting that colour induction is a retinal phenomenon.  相似文献   

4.
Specificity of cortico-cortical connections in monkey visual system   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
M S Livingstone  D H Hubel 《Nature》1983,304(5926):531-534
When the primate primary visual cortex, area 17, is stained for the mitochondrial enzyme cytochrome oxidase, it shows a striking polka-dot pattern (cytochrome oxidase blobs). Area 18, the second visual area, shows a cytochrome-oxidase pattern of coarse alternating thick and thin stripes running perpendicular to the 17-18 border and separated by lighter (interstripe) regions. Here we show that the thin cytochrome oxidase stripes, and possibly also the thick stripes, in area 18 receive projections specifically from the blobs in area 17, and that the interstripe regions of 18 receive projections from the interblob matrix of area 17. This indicates a specificity of cortico-cortical connections far exceeding the demands of topographical mapping. Together with our physiological results, it suggests that within the pathway from area 17 to area 18 different kinds of information may be handled separately and in parallel.  相似文献   

5.
P Thompson  G Latchford 《Nature》1986,320(6062):525-526
McCollough reported that following adaptation to (say) a red and black pattern of vertical stripes, alternating every few seconds with a green and black pattern of horizontal stripes, an orientation-contingent colour after-effect is observed when black and white gratings are viewed. Vertical gratings are tinged with green and horizontal gratings with pink. We have exploited colour constancy, the tendency for objects to appear constant in hue despite large changes in the spectral composition of the illuminant, to examine whether the colours observed on the McCollough effect test gratings are determined by the wavelength composition of the adaptation patterns or by their perceived colour. The key to this approach can be illustrated by Edwin Land's elegant demonstrations of colour constancy using 'Mondrian' displays. By embedding the adapting grating that is used to induce the McCollough effect within a Mondrian we show that the effect depends upon the wavelength of light coming from the grating, rather than the perceived colour.  相似文献   

6.
Photonic engineering. Aphrodite's iridescence   总被引:8,自引:0,他引:8  
The most intense colours displayed in nature result from either multilayer reflectors or linear diffraction gratings. Here we investigate the spectacular iridescence of a spine (notoseta) from the sea mouse Aphrodita sp. (Polychaeta: Aphroditidae). The spine normally appears to be deep red in colour, but when light is incident perpendicular to the axis of the spine, different colours are seen as stripes running parallel to the axis of the spine; over a range of smaller incident angles, the complete visible spectrum is reflected with a reflectivity of 100% to the human eye. The simple structure responsible for this effect is a remarkable example of photonic engineering by a living organism.  相似文献   

7.
Induction of visual orientation modules in auditory cortex   总被引:13,自引:0,他引:13  
Sharma J  Angelucci A  Sur M 《Nature》2000,404(6780):841-847
Modules of neurons sharing a common property are a basic organizational feature of mammalian sensory cortex. Primary visual cortex (V1) is characterized by orientation modules--groups of cells that share a preferred stimulus orientation--which are organized into a highly ordered orientation map. Here we show that in ferrets in which retinal projections are routed into the auditory pathway, visually responsive neurons in 'rewired' primary auditory cortex are also organized into orientation modules. The orientation tuning of neurons within these modules is comparable to the tuning of cells in V1 but the orientation map is less orderly. Horizontal connections in rewired cortex are more patchy and periodic than connections in normal auditory cortex, but less so than connections in V1. These data show that afferent activity has a profound influence on diverse components of cortical circuitry, including thalamocortical and local intracortical connections, which are involved in the generation of orientation tuning, and long-range horizontal connections, which are important in creating an orientation map.  相似文献   

8.
S Shipp  S Zeki 《Nature》1985,315(6017):322-325
V5 and V4 are areas of macaque monkey prestriate visual cortex that are specialized for involvement in different aspects of visual perception, namely motion for V5 (refs 1-4) and colour vision, with other possible functions, for V4 (refs 2, 5-9). Thus, it is unlikely that they should be fed the same information for further processing, yet both receive a strong input from patches of the upper layers of V2 (refs 10, 11), the area immediately adjoining the primary visual cortex, V1. V2, however, seems to comprise functionally distinct subregions, which can be revealed by staining the tissue for the mitochondrial enzyme cytochrome oxidase. Here we report that V4 and V5 are connected with separate cytochrome oxidase-defined subregions of V2, suggesting that cortical pathways dealing with motion and colour perception are segregated in their passage through V2, and reinforcing evidence for functional specialization in the visual cortex.  相似文献   

9.
利用差值转移的方法证明了,如果g(G)≥4则有X′a≤Δ(G)+4.图G=(V,E)是简单图,映射C:E→[k],被称作是图G的一个无圈k边染色.如果任意相邻的两个边染有不同的颜色,以及图G中不含有2-色圈,换句话说即图G中任何染两种颜色的边的导出子图是一棵森林.  相似文献   

10.
Dominy NJ  Lucas PW 《Nature》2001,410(6826):363-366
Trichromatic colour vision, characterized by three retinal photopigments tuned to peak wavelengths of approximately 430 nm, approximately 535 nm and approximately 562 nm (refs 1, 2), has evolved convergently in catarrhine primates and one genus of New World monkey, the howlers (genus Alouatta). This uniform capacity to discriminate red-green colours, which is not found in other mammals, has been proposed as advantageous for the long-range detection of either ripe fruits or young leaves (which frequently flush red in the tropics) against a background of mature foliage. Here we show that four trichromatic primate species in Kibale Forest, Uganda, eat leaves that are colour discriminated only by red-greenness, a colour axis correlated with high protein levels and low toughness. Despite their divergent digestive systems, these primates have no significant interspecific differences in leaf colour selection. In contrast, eaten fruits were generally discriminated from mature leaves on both red-green and yellow-blue channels and also by their luminance, with a significant difference between chimpanzees and monkeys in fruit colour choice. Our results implicate leaf consumption, a critical food resource when fruit is scarce, as having unique value in maintaining trichromacy in catarrhines.  相似文献   

11.
本文以色彩构成理论为依据,找出小配色模纹和较大配色模纹的不同配色所产生外观效应的规律,应用色彩构成规律使织物组织的设计尽可能符合美学观点。  相似文献   

12.
本文通过对比法探讨了英汉两种语言中色彩词的差异,说明由于语言习惯、历史背景、传统风俗等方面的差异,表示颜色的方法和用词有很多不同,对同一颜色的理解也有很大差别。因此,了解并掌握这此颜色词及所构成词汇的差异,对中外文化交流及翻译实践研究有一定的实际意义。  相似文献   

13.
Dynamic coding of behaviourally relevant stimuli in parietal cortex.   总被引:12,自引:0,他引:12  
Louis J Toth  John A Assad 《Nature》2002,415(6868):165-168
A general function of cerebral cortex is to allow the flexible association of sensory stimuli with specific behaviours. Many neurons in parietal, prefrontal and motor cortical areas are activated both by particular movements and by sensory cues that trigger these movements, suggesting a role in linking sensation to action. For example, neurons in the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) encode both the location of visual stimuli and the direction of saccadic eye movements. LIP is not believed to encode non-spatial stimulus attributes such as colour. Here we investigated whether LIP would encode colour if colour was behaviourally linked to the eye movement. We trained monkeys to make an eye movement in one of two directions based alternately on the colour or location of a visual cue. When cue colour was relevant for directing eye movement, we found a substantial fraction of LIP neurons selective for cue colour. However, when cue location was relevant, colour selectivity was virtually absent in LIP. These results demonstrate that selectivity of cortical neurons can change as a function of the required behaviour.  相似文献   

14.
地图作为空间信息的载体与传输工具,与色彩(之称颜色)的关系十分密切。为了深入理解色彩的基本同性,逐步实施地图的科学用色,本文论述了色彩基本理论与地图科学用色的关系,说明了对色彩进行测量、标定的方法,探讨了色彩数据化与地图设色的关系。  相似文献   

15.
室内色彩环境是一个人为设计的、三维的装饰色彩世界 ,其配色方法离不开装饰色彩配色原理的指导 ,并且在整体与局部的色彩关系上有着一定的特殊性  相似文献   

16.
设G是一个图,G的全着色是一个映射π:V(G)YE(G)C,使得相关联或相邻的元素着不同色;G的所有全着色中,使得色数的最小者,称为G的全色数,记为χT(G);得到了几个特殊图的全色数  相似文献   

17.
Parallel processing of motion and colour information   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
T Carney  M Shadlen  E Switkes 《Nature》1987,328(6131):647-649
When the two eyes are confronted with sufficiently different versions of the visual environment, one or the other eye dominates perception in alternation. A similar situation may be created in the laboratory by presenting images to the left and right eyes which differ in orientation or colour. Although perception is dominated by one eye during rivalry, there are a number of instances in which visual processes nevertheless continue to integrate information from the suppressed eye. For example the interocular transfer of the motion after-effect is undiminished when induced during binocular rivalry. Thus motion information processing may occur in parallel with the rivalry process. Here we describe a novel example in which the visual system simultaneously exhibits binocular rivalry and vision that integrates signals from both eyes. This apparent contradiction is resolved by postulating parallel visual processes devoted to the analyses of colour and motion information. Counterphased gratings are viewed dichoptically such that for one eye the grating is composed of alternating yellow and black stripes (luminance) while for the other it is composed of alternating red and green stripes (chrominance). When the gratings are fused, a moving grating is perceived. A consistent direction of motion can only be achieved if left and right monocular signals are integrated by the nervous system. Yet the apparent colour of the binocular percept alternates between red-green and yellow-black. These observations demonstrate the segregation of processing by the early motion system from that affording the perception of colour. Although, in this stimulus, colour information in itself can play no part in the cyclopean perception of motion direction, colour is carried along perceptually (filled in) by the moving pattern which is integrated from both eyes.  相似文献   

18.
I M Blythe  J M Bromley  C Kennard  K H Ruddock 《Nature》1986,320(6063):619-621
Damage to the striate cortex usually causes blindness in those regions of the visual field which map to the area of neural damage. Nonetheless, there are reports that some patients with such damage can localize and perform certain visual discriminations between light stimuli presented within the 'blind' area of the visual field. Experiments on animals with different brain areas ablated suggest that visual function is served by two principal projection pathways from the retina. That to the striate cortex is primarily responsible for fine discrimination between stimulus parameters such as colour and spatial pattern, whereas that to the superior colliculus in the midbrain is responsible for visual localization of stimuli. The residual visual functions in patients with cortical damage are usually attributed to the non-striate retinal projection to the superior colliculus. We now present measurements of spatial discrimination in two observers with large visual field defects (scotomata) caused by damage to the striate cortical region. Both exhibit a near normal ability to discriminate displacements of targets when two lights are flashed sequentially in their defective visual field, but they are unable to discriminate spatial pattern or size. We argue that these results are consistent with the 'two visual systems' interpretation of ablation studies on non-human species.  相似文献   

19.
用单波长激光制作真彩色全息图   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
研究将已有的彩色技术与彩色印刷术相结合,在无全色光致抗蚀剂的情况下,利用单一波长的激光光源和现有的蓝敏光致抗蚀剂,采用密度编码分区记录方法,在同一干版的不同区域记录三原色掩模的菲涅耳全息图,白光再现下,获得真彩色全息图.  相似文献   

20.
工业设计的色彩量化处理   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
从色系的建立,色彩的标准化,色彩心理效应的表格化等方面讨论了建立色彩CAD软件模型的基础-色彩处理量化的可行性,并从目标色的选择和配制等方面提出量化方法的构想。  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号